Action-specific perception

Action-specific perception, or perception-action, is a psychological theory that people perceive their environment and events within it in terms of their ability to act.[1][2] This theory hence suggests that a person's capability to carry out a particular task affects how they perceive the different aspects and methods involved in that task.[1][2] For example, softball players who are hitting better see the ball as bigger.[3] Tennis players see the ball as moving slower when they successfully return the ball.[4] In the field of human-computer interaction, alterations in accuracy impact both the perception of size and time, while adjustments in movement speed impact the perception of distance.[5] Furthermore, the perceiver's intention to act is also critical; while the perceiver's ability to perform the intended action influences perception, the perceiver's abilities for unintended actions have little or no effect on perception.[6] Finally, the objective difficulty of the task appears to modulate size, distance, and time perception.[5]

Overview

Action-specific effects have been documented in a variety of contexts and with a variety of manipulations.[1] The original work was done on perceived slant of hills and perceived distance to targets. Hills look steeper and targets look farther away when wearing a heavy backpack.[7][8] In addition to walking, many other actions influence perception such as throwing, jumping, falling, reaching, grasping, kicking, hitting, blocking, and swimming. In addition to perceived slant and perceived distance, other aspects of perception are influenced by ability such as size, shape, height, and speed. These results have been documented in athletes such as softball players, golfers, tennis players, swimmers, and people skilled in parkour. However, a criticism would be that these action-specific effects on perception may surface only in extreme cases (e.g., professional athletes) or condition (e.g., steep hills). Recent evidence from virtual reality, indicated that these action-specific effects are observed in both "normal" conditions and average individuals.[5]

Background

The action-specific perception account has roots in Gibson's (1979) ecological approach to perception.[9] According to Gibson, the primary objects of perception are affordances, which are the possibilities for action. Affordances capture the mutual relationship between the environment and the perceiver. For example, a tall wall is a barrier to an elderly person but affords jumping-over to someone trained in parkour, or urban climbing. Like the ecological approach, the action-specific perception account favors the notion that perception involves processes that relate the environment to the perceiver's potential for action. Consequently, similar environments will look different, depending on the abilities of each perceiver. Since abilities change over time, an individual's perception of similar environments will also change as their abilities change.

Criticisms

The claim that activity and intention influence perception is controversial.[citation needed] These findings challenge traditional theories of perception, nearly all of which conceptualize perception as a process that provides an objective and behaviorally-independent representation of the environment.[attribution needed] The fact that the same environment looks different depending on the perceiver's abilities and intentions implies that perception is not behaviorally-neutral.

Alternative explanations for apparent action-specific effects have been proposed, most commonly that the perceiver's ability affects the perceiver's judgment about what they see, rather than affecting perception itself. In other words, perceivers see the world similarly but then report their impressions differently.[10]

Problems

Perception cannot be measured directly. Instead, researchers must rely on reports, judgments, and behaviors. However, many attempts have been made to resolve this issue. One technique is to use many different kinds of perceptual judgments.[1] For example, action-specific effects have been found when verbal reports and visual matching tasks.[clarification needed] Action-specific effects are also apparent with indirect measures such as perceived parallelism as a proxy for perceived distance. Action-specific effects have also been found when using action-based measures such as Blindwalking.[citation needed]

See also

References

  1. ^ a b c d Witt, J. K. (2011). Action's effect on perception. Current Directions in Psychological Science.
  2. ^ a b Proffitt, D. R. (2006). Embodied perception and the economy of action. Perspectives on Psychological Science.
  3. ^ Witt, J. K., & Proffitt, D.R. (2005). See the ball, hit the ball: Apparent ball size is correlated with batting average. Psychological Science, 16, 937-938.
  4. ^ Witt, J. K., & Sugovic, M. (2010). Performance and ease influence perceived speed. Perception, 39, 1341-1353.
  5. ^ a b c Kourtesis, Panagiotis; Vizcay, Sebastian; Marchal, Maud; Pacchierotti, Claudio; Argelaguet, Ferran (November 2022). "Action-Specific Perception & Performance on a Fitts's Law Task in Virtual Reality: The Role of Haptic Feedback". IEEE Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics. 28 (11): 3715–3726. arXiv:2207.07400. doi:10.1109/TVCG.2022.3203003. ISSN 1077-2626. PMID 36048989. S2CID 250607543.
  6. ^ Witt, J. K., Proffitt, D. R., & Epstein, W. (2010). How and when does action scale perception? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 1153-1160.
  7. ^ Bhalla, M., & Proffitt, D. R. (1999). Visual-Motor recalibration in geographical slant perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 25, 1076-1096.
  8. ^ Proffitt, D.R., Stefanucci, J., Banton, T., & Epstein, W. (2003). The role of effort in perceiving distance. Psychological Science, 14, 106-112.
  9. ^ Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception (Boston: Houghton Mifflin)
  10. ^ Loomis J M, Philbeck J W, (2008). Measuring spatial perception with spatial understanding and action”, in Embodiment, Ego-Space, and Action Eds. R L Klatzky, B MacWhinney, M Behrmann (Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press) pp. 1–44, here: p. 33.